Any type of research which violates the set ethics is termed unethical and forbidden. Borderline areas refer to boundaries which the researcher is not allowed to cross in research since it may cause repercussions on the subjects

RESPOND TO TEACHER (4-22-18)

Yvette

WednesdayApr 18 at 9pm

Manage Discussion Entry

Forbidden Research

Any type of research which violates the set ethics is termed unethical and forbidden. Borderline areas refer to boundaries which the researcher is not allowed to cross in research since it may cause repercussions on the subjects (APA, 2016).  Firstly, research whereby at the end of it the subjects may be negatively affected is not allowed. For example, in 2001, a research study testing on speech remedy was conducted at Lowa University using experimental and control groups. After the research, it was revealed that children who were subjected to negative psychoanalysis developed adverse mental effects while the rest reserved speech complications.

Secondly, a research study which is feared that the results may be disastrous is prohibited. It is dreadful in that it ends up affecting the participants or the environment where it was conducted. For example, subjects may end up experiencing depression, helplessness, and emotional disturbances.

Thirdly, in a case where animals are being used for experimentation, the experiment should not go past ethics realms. This may involve a case whereby the animals may end up being much disturbed, injured, convulsions or even cause their death. For example, a monkey study which was carried out in 1969 on drug trials left many nursing injuries, psychologically disturbed and others were feared to die in a short span of two weeks (Flick, 2014). In addition, exposing the participant to unfavorable condition or chemical which may have a negative impact on them is forbidden.

Fourthly, in any case, a participant wishes to withdraw his or her results from an experiment, there is no reason for pushing the subject to continue with the research. The subject’s move might have been propelled due to unfavorable conditions, and in this case, they should be permitted to halt. If the researcher compels the subject to continue for selfish gains, that is unethical and forbidden (Bell, 2014).

 

References

American Psychological Association. (2016). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct. American Psychologist

Flick, U. (2014). An introduction to qualitative research. Sage

Bell, J. (2014). Doing Your Research Project: A guide for first-time researchers. McGraw-Hill Education (UK).

 

 

 

Reply Reply to Comment

·

COLLAPSE SUBDISCUSSION Jean Henry

Jean Henry

ThursdayApr 19 at 3:26pm

Manage Discussion Entry

Hi Yvette

Wow! Okay, are you collecting these bonuses for a rainy day? This should really help you if you need a reprieve to redo an assignment or need a little leeway in terms of posting assignments for the rest of this week or week 5. In any case, you’ve just earned another bonus! Feel free to use it to request a two day late exception for any week 1,2, 3, 4, or 5 assignment or for a chance to redo one initial post or paper from those weeks. In any case, it is just the two of us. So, feel free to respond to me once or more. That is fine.:)

In any case, we certainly do need to be mindful of participant rights such as the right to withdraw, human protections, appropriate professional conduct of a researcher, etc. Related to those too, deception is important to consider. As we know, interestingly, deception is possible in scientific research credibly, considered ethically appropriate in some situations, and even does have benefits when used properly (both in terms of research and arguably also in terms of the welfare of the participant).

According to the American Psychological Association (2017) Ethics Code, deception can and is used in research credibly at times. If approved to be used though, it needs to be J.E.N.: Justified (a clear experimentally sound rationale for the need for deception), Explained (participants need to know that deception in some way may occur), and Non-harmful (does not pose any notable risk, harm, etc. to participants). For example, having Confederates (essentially, fake study participants) could be Justified (J). Confederates could also be Explained (E) in some ways without disclosing everything that would give them away or harm the study and still be approved. In addition, arguably in most situations, having Confederates would not harm (N) anyone. This is just one of many possible examples.

By the way, a great example of the use of deception with Confederates is the famous study in psychology, the 1973 Darley and Batson Good Samaritan study, which addressed dispositional and situational factors that play a role when people are immersed in an experience (Brooks, 2012). Simply stated, the researchers wanted to know if personal factors and/or situational/environmental factors influenced people’s decisions and actions.

So, they used a Confederate who acted like an injured individual who needed help. People largely walked by, even those for whom that behavior might not be expected (e.g., seminary students, etc.). If the researchers had let others know the Confederate was role-playing, the study could not have happened. It arguably didn’t hurt anyone to have that confederate there, and the study even today would have likely been approved. That said, if you were a researcher and needed to conduct a study using deception in some way, would you do it? If so or not, why or how?

Dr. Henry

References

American Psychological Association. (2017). Ethical principles of psychologists and

code of conduct. Retrieved from http://apa.org/ethics/code/index.asp

Brooks, R. (2012). Lessons from social psychology for complex operations.

Georgetown Public Law and Legal Theory Research Paper No. 13-043. Retrieved

from http://scholarship.law.georgetown.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=2109&context=facpub

The clinical setting is that of a Family and Veteran Clinic that offers a Home-Based Program that provides treatment for patients with Post Traumatic Disorder.

RESPOND TO CLASSMATE YVETTE(4-22-18)

Yvette Lunday

WednesdayApr 18 at 12:19pm

Manage Discussion Entry

Neuropsychological Practice

The clinical setting is that of a Family and Veteran Clinic that offers a Home-Based Program that provides treatment for patients with Post Traumatic Disorder. The type of service offered is an outpatient service which opens from 8 am to 5 pm where expertise from different disciplines are provided for treatment for patients who suffer from stress-related conditions and mood disorders. Family and child psychiatrists and psychologists, nurse specialists and OIF combat veterans. The outreach team of veterans is also involved in the clinical engagement to serve as the point person for the vet during the care treatment and maintain their retention. The goal of the working model is to engage veteran members and have their families’ access care. It involves highlighting the clinical engagement and the treatment opportunities offered to the patient (Allard & Rebbeca, 2015, p.99). An instance case can be a marine veteran who experiences relationship distress with her wife because of extreme withdrawal and emotional numbing. The children as well experience academic difficulties because of poor implementation of homework plans. The treatment engagement would be to induce parenting guidance and strengthening the parenting bond which will trigger towards re-establishing his role and authority. There will also be assessment system will be designed to probe interrelationships between the veteran and family levels of functioning to improve its outcomes that overcome the barriers to accessing health care for the military families.  The central attribution to the program model is to decrease distress as a way of improving functioning within the family. Some of the challenges are missing appointments from the patient and family members. The derailed treatment would need an increase in engagement that would have to reduce adverse outcomes of distress within relationships which would have been dealing with earlier on.

Reference

Allard M& Rebecca W., (2015), Three Generational Model- A Family Systems Network for the

Assessment and Treatment for Veterans, Journal of Psychology Research and Practice,

46:2, p. 97-106

Innovations in Correctional Assessment and Treatment

To prepare for this Discussion:

  • Review the article, “Innovations in Correctional Assessment and Treatment.” Focus on specific areas of mental health assessment and treatment in correctional settings, how these areas are studied, and the results.
  • Think about how you might apply the results of this study in a correctional setting.
  • Review this week’s DVD program, “Application of Psychological Research – Correctional Settings.” Consider the types of forensic psychology research questions that are relevant to correctional settings.
  • Using the Walden Library, select a psychological research study conducted in a correctional setting.
  • Review the section(s) on methods and results of the research study you selected and think about what you believe to be the most important contributions of the study for use in correctional settings.
  • Consider how a forensic psychology professional might apply the results of this study in a correctional setting.

With these thoughts in mind:

Post by Day 4 a brief description of the research study you selected. Then, explain the parts of this study that you believe might be important for forensic psychology professionals working in correctional settings. Finally, explain how a forensic psychology professional might use the results of the study in a correctional setting.

Be sure to support your postings and responses with specific references to the Learning Resources.

Differential effects of a body image exposure session on smoking urge between physically active and sedentary female smokers

Read the article “Differential effects of a body image exposure session on smoking urge between physically active and sedentary female smokers,”and  identify the research questions and/or hypotheses as they are stated.  Consider the following questions: What are the variables (sample sizes,  population, treatments, etc.)?  What are the inferential statistics used  in this article?  Were the proper steps of hypothesis testing followed?

Write a two- to three-page paper presenting the information listed  below. Include a title page and reference page in APA style.  Cite any  references made to the article within the body of the paper in APA  style. Your paper should begin with an introductory paragraph (including  a thesis statement) and end with a concluding paragraph summarizing the  major points made in the body of the paper and reaffirming the thesis.   When writing the article critique, your paper must:

  1. Determine what question(s) the authors are trying to answer by doing this research
  2. Determine the hypothesis being tested and the concepts that were applied in this process.
  3. Evaluate the article and critique the statistical analysis employed in the study.
    • Would you have included more and/or different variables? Explain your answer.
  4. Examine the assumptions and limitations of the statistical study.
    • What would you have done differently in this case? Why?
  5. Identify how the authors applied statistical testing to the problem.
  6. Interpret the findings of the author(s) using statistical concepts.

You may access the Critical Thinking Community (Links to an external site.)Links to an external site. website for tips on how to formulate your thoughts and discussion of these questions in a logical and meaningful manner.
Writing the Article Critique
The Assignment:

  1. Must be two to three double-spaced pages in length (excluding title  and reference pages), and formatted according to APA style as outlined  in the Ashford Writing Center.
  2. Must include a title page with the following:
    1. Title of paper
    2. Student’s name
    3. Course name and number
    4. Instructor’s name
    5. Date submitted
  3. Must document all sources in APA style, as outlined in the Ashford Writing Center.
  4. Must include a separate reference page, formatted according to APA style as outlined in the Ashford Writing Center.

designing a Counseling Group from start to finish.

THE GROUP TOPIC WILL BE CHILDREN OF DIVORCE

Throughout this course, you will be designing a Counseling Group from start to finish. The assignment will be broken into four parts, which are due at different intervals in the course. For the three-part assignment, choose from the following group types (If you are in the addiction counseling program, select an addiction group):

  1. Children of divorce
  2. Children of addicts
  3. Adult Survivors of abuse (could be sexual or physical)
  4. Treatment for adults who are addicts
  5. Grief recovery
  6. Teens struggling with gender dysphoria (sexual identity)
  7. Teen support group (parents who are addicts)
  8. Relationship building (homosexual and heterosexual – there does not have to be a division because a group is a group)
  9. Domestic violence group for nonoffenders
  10. Parenting group

Identify what type of group you will design by selecting one of the group types listed above.

Write a 300-600-word summary of your selected group.

Be sure to include the following in your summary:

  1. Group type
  2. Why you chose this type of group
  3. Population serving
  4. Number of sessions
  5. Number of participants
  6. Goals of the counseling group
  7. A minimum of two scholarly references, not including the course textbook

APA style is not required, but solid academic writing is expected.

This assignment uses a scoring guide. Please review the guide prior to beginning the assignment to become familiar with the expectations for successful completion.

Analyze the physical, cognitive, social and personality aspects of abnormal psychology and implications across the life span

Assignment 8 Begin Final Research Paper.
This assignment MUST be typed in APA style, and must be written at graduate level English.
Prepare a comprehensive Final Research Paper in which you choose an  area of research that is relevant and pertinent to the current diagnosis  and treatment of mental illness in accordance to the DSM-5. Your topic  must include current perspectives in diagnosis, assessment and  treatment, including an integrative discussion of traditional  psychological research and neuroscience.
Select a minimum of eight (8) current research articles taken from  scholarly journals (online or hard copy) on your selected topic. You are  encouraged to utilize the Calsouthern Library to access evidence-based  resources. You may use the bibliography located under Resources in your  course syllabus.
Do not use the course text or other texts for this assignment.
 This is a research-based paper.
Research must be no more than 3-5 years old.
Your paper must be 10-12 pages plus a title and reference page
Assignment Outcomes:

Analyze the physical, cognitive, social and personality aspects of abnormal psychology and implications across the life span

Examine the major diagnostic domains and specific criteria  associated with DSM-5 disorders. Evaluate legal and ethical issues in  mental health treatment

Develop an awareness of diversity and cross-cultural perspectives in abnormal psychology.

Explore available treatment and interdisciplinary services for community members experiencing mental health disorders.

Identify the barriers associated with seeking and receiving  therapeutic services. Demonstrate ethical behavior in the use of  technology

schizophrenia that addresses potential depression and suicidality.

Create a 1,200 word safety plan for a client similar to Ted, who had been diagnosed with schizophrenia that addresses potential depression and suicidality.

Include the following in your safety plan:

    What symptoms would a client with schizophrenia exhibit? What symptoms did Ted display?

    How would you have addressed Ted’s symptoms related to delusions, hallucinations, and depression?

    What other diagnosis might Ted have been misdiagnosed with and why?

    Describe which theories would have been most effective and which theories would have been least effective for treating Ted’s schizophrenia. Explain your rationale.

    Describe treatment options for addressing all of Ted’s symptoms.

    Explain how a client’s religious or spiritual beliefs come into play during the process of dealing with depression and suicide.

    Include at least five scholarly references in addition to the textbook in your paper.

Prepare this assignment according to the guidelines found in the APA Style Guide, located in the Student Success Center.

This assignment uses a rubric. Please review the rubric prior to beginning the assignment to become familiar with the expectations for successful completion.

You are required to submit this assignment to Turnitin. Refer to the directions in the Student Success Center.

This benchmark assignment assesses the following programmatic competency: 1.4: Demonstrate knowledge and skill in working with unique counseling populations.

2

Case Study: Ted

A single man of 40 years of age named Ted cut his carotid artery at home. He had suffered from chronic schizophrenia, dominated by paranoid symptoms, for 20 years. During his illness, Ted had spent a total of 12 years in mental hospitals; individual hospitalizations had varied in duration. While he was hospitalized, his bizarre delusions of altered body states and his experiences of being controlled by external, often invisible, agents rapidly disappeared. He had death wishes and suicidal thoughts since the onset of his schizophrenia. Death wishes also stopped soon after hospitalization.

Over the years, opinion about Ted changed and his condition began to be regarded as hopeless. He was difficult to treat; he accused personnel, was unreliable, acted pretentiously, and reacted by acting out. Four years before committing suicide, he had to be transferred to another mental hospital. Two years before his death, he was transferred to a halfway house belonging to the hospital, because the staff feared that his dependence on the hospital might become excessive. After his transfer to outpatient care, his suicidal tendencies increased. Six months before committing suicide, he lost his long-term nurse. Subsequent treatment consisted of occasional office visits with a psychologist or psychiatrist.

Just before committing suicide, Ted tried to enter the hospital where he had been during the initial phases of his illness. He had suffered increasingly for a few months from paranoid fears of being murdered. He threatened to commit suicide unless he was admitted to the hospital, but the threat was considered demonstrative and hospitalization was brief.

The day before he committed suicide, he visited his childhood home and became afraid that a group of men had surrounded the house. He repeated his wish to enter a mental hospital. During his final night, his state changed. According to his father, the Ted was exceptionally calm on the day of his death. The father said, “He no longer seemed afraid of anything.”

Adapted from:

Saarinen, P. I., Lehtonen, J., & Lönnqvist, J. (1999). Suicide risk in schizophrenia: An analysis of 17 consecutive suicides. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 25, 533-542.

Evidence That Infants Are Not Afraid of Heights

Current Directions in Psychological Science 2014, Vol. 23(1) 60 –66 © The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0963721413498895 cdps.sagepub.com

Are human infants afraid of heights? At first blush, the answer appears to be a resounding “yes.” More than half a century ago, E. J. Gibson and Walk (1960) reported that crawling infants readily cross a visible surface of support but avoid crawling over an apparent, meter-high drop- off. To ensure infants’ safety, researchers tested babies on a glass-covered precipice, dubbed a “visual cliff” because the drop-off was only illusory (see Figure 1a), rather than a real cliff from which foolhardy infants could fall. The visual cliff is a classic paradigm in developmental psy- chology; the image of an infant peering into a checker- board-patterned abyss is among the most famous icons in developmental science. Furthermore, the basic find- ings are well known to the thousands of students who have sat through introductory courses in developmental psychology, experimental psychology, or perception. Perhaps because the paradigm has such common-sense appeal and apparent face validity (everyone can under- stand the importance of avoiding locomotion over a large drop-off, and most of us have experienced some sort of fear of heights), avoidance and fear are commonly conflated.

Subsequent researchers have expanded on E. J. Gibson and Walk’s (1960) original findings and have introduced a few caveats regarding the role of locomotor experience (Adolph & Kretch, 2012). For example, human infants (and altricial animals, such as kittens) require several weeks of self-produced locomotor experience before they avoid the deep side of the visual cliff (Bertenthal, Campos, & Barrett, 1984; Held & Hein, 1963). Likewise,

on a real cliff, a large gap in the surface of support, or an impossibly steep slope (see Figures 1b–1d), infants plunge right over the edge unless they have many weeks of locomotor experience (Adolph, 1997, 2000; Adolph, Berger, & Leo, 2011; Kretch & Adolph, 2013a). These apparatuses have no safety glass; experimenters catch infants if they begin to fall. Moreover, the apparatuses are continuously adjustable so that researchers can precisely assess the correspondence between infants’ attempts and their actual abilities. Over weeks of crawling and walk- ing, infants become increasingly accurate, attempting drop-offs, gaps, and slopes within their abilities and avoiding those beyond their abilities.

What then is the role of locomotor experience in facili- tating adaptive avoidance responses? The best known hypothesis is that self-produced locomotion leads to fear of heights, and fear leads to avoidance (Bertenthal et al., 1984; Campos et al., 2000; Campos, Hiatt, Ramsay, Henderson, & Svejda, 1978). In support of this account, crawling infants show accelerated heart rate—a standard index of fear—when placed on the deep side of the visual cliff, but prelocomotor infants do not (Campos, Bertenthal, & Kermoian, 1992; Campos et al., 1978). Likewise, kittens, goats, and other animals show stereo- typed fear responses—such as freezing and backing up

498895 CDP10.1177/0963721413498895Adolph et al.Fear of Heights in Infants? research-article2014

Corresponding Author: Karen E. Adolph, New York University, 6 Washington Place, Room 415, New York, NY 10003 E-mail: karen.adolph@nyu.edu

Fear of Heights in Infants?

Karen E. Adolph1, Kari S. Kretch1, and Vanessa LoBue2 1New York University and 2Rutgers University

Abstract Based largely on the famous “visual cliff” paradigm, conventional wisdom is that crawling infants avoid crossing the brink of a dangerous drop-off because they are afraid of heights. However, recent research suggests that the conventional wisdom is wrong. Avoidance and fear are conflated, and there is no compelling evidence to support fear of heights in human infants. Infants avoid crawling or walking over an impossibly high drop-off because they perceive affordances for locomotion—the relations between their own bodies and skills and the relevant properties of the environment that make an action such as descent possible or impossible.

Keywords emotion, locomotion, affordances

at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

http://cdp.sagepub.com/

Fear of Heights in Infants? 61

Fig. 1. Apparatuses used to test infants’ reactions to heights. (a) Visual cliff. The surface on the deep side is 102 centi- meters below the centerboard, and the surface on the shallow side is 3 centimeters below the centerboard. (b) Actual cliff used in Kretch and Adolph’s (2013a) study. Height adjusts from 0 to 90 centimeters in 1-centimeter increments. (c) Adjustable gap used in Adolph’s (2000) study. Gap width adjusts from 0 to 90 centimeters in 2-centimeter increments. (d) Adjustable slope used in Adolph’s (1997) study. Steepness adjusts from 0 to 90 degrees in 4-degree increments. (e–f) Bridge apparatus from Kretch and Adolph’s (2013b) study shown in the large (71-centimeter) and small (17-centimeter) drop-off conditions. Bridge width adjusts from 2 to 60 centimeters in 2-centimeter increments.

at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

http://cdp.sagepub.com/

62 Adolph et al.

with stiff forelegs—when they are placed directly onto the glass or pushed over the edge onto the deep side. Although the placing paradigm is more like being thrown off a cliff than exploring the view from the edge, the common interpretation is that fear of heights mediates avoidance.

Evidence That Infants Are Not Afraid of Heights

Despite half a century of undergraduates learning that infants avoid a drop-off because they are afraid of heights, several sources of evidence suggest that this idea is not accurate. First, researchers have no corroborating evi- dence of fear. The evidence for fear of heights is the avoidance response itself. The argument is circular and goes something like this: Infants avoid because they are fearful; we know they are fearful because they avoid. Physiological measures, such as heart rate, do not pro- vide independent corroboration that fear mediates avoid- ance. Two weeks of crawling experience was sufficient to elicit accelerated heart rate when infants were placed on the deep side of the visual cliff but not to elicit avoid- ance when infants were allowed to cross (Campos et al., 1992). The same infants with pounding hearts in the plac- ing paradigm crawled straight over the glass in the cross- ing paradigm (Ueno, Uchiyama, Campos, Dahl, & Anderson, 2011). Therefore, accelerated heart rate may reflect arousal, not fear.

Similarly, although one might imagine that infants dis- play other indices of fear when they avoid a drop-off, they do not. Infants who avoid crawling over the deep side of the visual cliff ‘‘do not show prototypic fear expressions. Indeed, they often smile!’’ (Saarni, Campos, Camras, & Witherington, 2006, p. 231; also see Sorce, Emde, Campos, & Klinnert, 1985). Infants appear to enjoy the problem of deciding how to cope with obstacles. Likewise, on steep slopes, infants’ facial expressions and vocalizations are primarily positive or neutral, not nega- tive, regardless of whether they go over the edge or avoid (Adolph, Tamis-LeMonda, Ishak, Karasik, & Lobo, 2008; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2008). Furthermore, there is no increase in negative expressions, such as crying or cling- ing, with infants’ age or locomotor experience. Thus, some researchers have argued that the only valid index of fear on the visual cliff is refusal to crawl or walk over the brink because facial expressions and heart rate are not reliable indicators (Saarni et al., 2006).

A second line of evidence against the notion that fear mediates avoidance is infants’ proximity to the edge. Although readers might assume that “avoidance” means that infants stay away from the drop-off, it does not. In every paradigm—the visual cliff, real cliffs, gaps, slopes, and bridges—infants spend most of each trial right at the

edge of the drop-off, exploring possibilities for locomo- tion by stretching an arm toward the bottom of the preci- pice or by rocking back and forth at the brink (Adolph, 1997, 2000; Kretch & Adolph, 2013a, 2013b; Ueno et al., 2011; Walk, 1966; Walk & Gibson, 1961; Witherington, Campos, Anderson, Lejeune, & Seah, 2005). In fact, on the visual cliff, infants do not get scored as crossing until they have placed all four limbs onto the safety glass (Witherington et al., 2005). On a real cliff or gap, they would have fallen. Moreover, in every paradigm, infants who avoid crawling or walking over the drop-off are likely to attempt descent using alternative methods of locomotion, such as scuttling along the side walls of the visual cliff as well as backing or sliding down real cliffs and slopes (Adolph, 1997; Campos et al., 1978; Kretch & Adolph, 2013a; Witherington et al., 2005). Thus, “avoid- ant” infants refuse to attempt crossing in their typical mode of locomotion, but they do not avoid proximity to the drop-off and do not typically avoid descent if alterna- tives are available. Their behaviors at the brink provide evidence of adaptive, flexible responding, not fear of heights.

A third line of evidence against fear as the critical mediator is that infants show no evidence that they understand the different consequences of falling from different drop-off heights (Kretch & Adolph, 2013b). Adults are more leery of falling from a larger, more dan- gerous height than a smaller one, but infants are not. Both crawling and walking infants carefully scale attempts to cross bridges (see Figures 1e–1f) to the width of the bridge, indicating that they accurately perceive the prob- ability of falling. However, attempts to cross, gait modifi- cations, and exploratory behavior are identical on bridges spanning a large 71-centimeter drop-off (infants’ standing height) and a small 17-centimeter drop-off (infants’ knee height), meaning that infants do not consider the severity of a potential fall when deciding whether to cross, and they are not more reticent to cross a large drop-off than a small one.

A fourth source of evidence to argue against fear of heights is that infants’ actions at the edge of a precipice depend on the constraints of the test situation. In several studies, walking infants treated the same degree of slope differently, depending on whether they were wearing a lead- or feather-weighted vest (Adolph & Avolio, 2000) or rubber- versus Teflon-soled shoes (Adolph, Karasik, & Tamis-LeMonda, 2010). They correctly attempted to walk down steep slopes (with a larger drop-off) while wearing the feather-weighted vest or the rubber-soled shoes but refused to walk down shallow slopes (with a smaller drop-off) in the lead-weighted vest or slippery-soled shoes, where their abilities were diminished. Infants accurately reassessed the situation when constraints changed from trial to trial. If fear were mediating infants’

at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

http://cdp.sagepub.com/

Fear of Heights in Infants? 63

responses, they should have treated the drop-offs simi- larly across conditions.

A final argument against fear of heights is that adap- tive responding at the edge of a drop-off depends on whether infants face the obstacle in a newly acquired posture or an old, familiar posture. In one study, infants in an experienced sitting posture refused to span deep gaps that were slightly too large relative to their balance abilities; however, in a novice crawling posture, the same infants repeatedly plunged head first into the precipice (Adolph, 2000). Similarly, novice walkers stepped right into impossibly wide gaps (Adolph et al., 2011). In an experienced crawling posture, infants refused to crawl down impossibly steep slopes or cliffs; however, in a novice walking posture, they walked over the edge (Adolph, 1997; Adolph et al., 2008; Kretch & Adolph, 2013a). Specificity of learning between earlier and later developing postures was so robust that infants alternated between avoiding and plunging on consecutive trials when the experimenter started them in an experienced or novice posture (Adolph, 1997). If locomotor experi- ence teaches infants fear of heights, adaptive avoidance responses should not depend on the posture in which infants are tested.

Why Do Babies Avoid a Drop-Off?

So why do infants avoid a drop-off? Furthermore, what do infants learn from locomotor experience that facili- tates adaptive responding at the edge of a precipice? Answering these questions requires an apparatus more modifiable than the visual cliff. The standard visual cliff has only two drop-off heights, the shallow and the deep side, and the safety glass precludes assessment of infants’ actual abilities because both sides are safe for locomo- tion. In contrast, on a real cliff with continuously adjust- able drop-off height, researchers can assess infants’ attempts to cross relative to their actual ability to navigate the drop-off. For experienced crawling and walking infants, a small drop-off—13 centimeters—is simply a step: If infants attempt to crawl or walk over the edge, they will succeed. However, for novice walkers, the same 13-centimeter drop-off is essentially a cliff: If they attempt to walk, they will fall (Kretch & Adolph, 2013a). The dis- tinction between a stepping-off place and a falling-off place depends on the fit between infants’ physical capa- bilities and the relevant environmental properties—what J. J. Gibson (1979) termed an affordance. On this account, infants decide whether to cross or avoid a drop-off by detecting information for affordances (e.g., limb length, muscle strength, and balance control relative to drop-off height). What infants learn from locomotor experience is how to perceive affordances for locomotion (Adolph, 1997; Adolph & Robinson, 2013; Kretch & Adolph, 2013a).

In her later writings, E. J. Gibson (E. J. Gibson, 1991; E. J. Gibson et al., 1987; E. J. Gibson & Schmuckler, 1989) reinterpreted findings from the visual cliff in terms of perception of affordances (Adolph & Kretch, 2012). E. J. Gibson (1982) did not equate avoidance with fear, and she did not believe that fear necessarily accompanied perception of affordances:

[Affordances] are not the attachment to a perception of feelings of pleasantness or unpleasantness. They are information for behavior that is of some potential utility to the animal . . . I doubt that a mountain goat peering over a steep crag is afraid or charged with any kind of emotion; he simply does not step off. (p. 65)

Although the evidence does not support an account based on fear, it is consistent with an affordance account. Infants explore a precipice to generate information for affordances. Exploration increased on more challenging cliffs, gaps, slopes, and bridges as attempts to crawl and walk decreased (Adolph, 1997, 2000; Kretch & Adolph, 2013a, 2013b). Indeed, on the visual cliff, infants patted the glass with their hands, leaned their weight on the glass, and laid their face on it (Ueno et al., 2011; Walk, 1966; Walk & Gibson, 1961; Witherington et al., 2005). After one trial, they figured out the illusion, and crossed the deep side (Campos et al., 1978; Walk, 1966). When experienced crawlers and walkers were tested on modifi- able apparatuses with no safety glass, they rarely erred. Attempts to crawl or walk were matched to their abili- ties—they only attempted risky obstacles that were within 1 to 3 centimeters of their actual ability on real cliffs, gaps, and bridges and that were within 2 to 6 degrees of slant on slopes (Adolph, 1997, 2000; Adolph et al., 2008; Kretch & Adolph, 2013a, 2013b). Moreover, experienced infants retained impressive accuracy when their abilities were altered with weighted shoulder packs or slippery shoes (Adolph & Avolio, 2000; Adolph et al., 2010). Such finely attuned perception of affordances requires many weeks of locomotor experience because infants must identify the relevant parameters for their new action sys- tems and learn to calibrate the settings of those parame- ters under varying conditions. Infants fail to show transfer from earlier to later developing postures because affor- dances and the information to specify the relevant rela- tions are different for sitting, crawling, cruising, and walking (Adolph & Robinson, 2013).

Unanswered Questions

What would constitute convincing evidence that infants are afraid of heights? Although infant fear is a controver- sial topic because of considerable individual variation

at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

http://cdp.sagepub.com/

64 Adolph et al.

(Saarni et al., 2006), infant distress is not. Unfortunately for parents, infants are quite adept at displaying negative affect. In the laboratory, infants in the same age range as those who avoid drop-offs are also capable of negative affect. When approached by a stranger, for example, some 8- to 22-month-old infants produce negative affect in facial expressions and vocalizations; they may also withdraw from the stimulus, cling to their mothers, show accelerated heart rate, and stiffen their bodies (e.g., Sroufe, 1977; Waters, Matas, & Sroufe, 1975). Thus, in addition to accelerated heart rate, evidence for fear of heights should include converging evidence, such as negative affect and withdrawal from the edge of the drop-off. However, infants do the opposite: Their facial expressions and vocalizations are positive or neutral, and they spend most of their time exploring at the edge of the drop-off and finding alternative means of crossing.

So when does fear of heights develop, and how is it acquired? To our knowledge, there are no longitudinal data to address this question. Previous work suggests that acquiring fear of heights does not necessitate direct condi- tioning experiences, such as a traumatic fall (Menzies & Clarke, 1993; Poulton, Davies, Menzies, Langley, & Silva, 1998). Indirect pathways may include observational learning or transmission of negative verbal information (Rachman, 1977). Individual differences in temperament or trait anxiety might also play a role by making some children more likely to acquire fear of heights than others (Mineka & Zinbarg, 2006).

A final question for future research is whether infants may show heightened sensitivity to other stimuli, without displaying evidence of fear. Fear of snakes and spiders, for example, is common in adults, but recent research indicates that infants and young children do not show fear of snakes or spiders (LoBue, 2013). Instead, they display positive or neutral affect, and they approach, rather than withdraw, from photographs, videos, and live snakes and spiders. However, infants and young children do show heightened visual sensitivity to snakes and spi- ders, akin to their heightened attention to drop-offs. Possibly, early perceptual sensitivity to stimuli, such as heights and snakes, facilitates fear learning later in devel- opment (LoBue, 2013).

Conclusions: Who Is Afraid of Heights?

Although infants, children, and adults can perceive affor- dances for locomotion, fear of heights is not universal at any age. Some sensation seekers enjoy the thrill of heights and seek out activities such as mountain climbing, para- sailing, or skydiving (Salassa & Zapala, 2009; Zuckerman, 1983). Others have more trepidations: About 30% of adults report nonclinical height fear, and another 5%

have full-blown height phobia (Agras, Sylvester, & Oliveau, 1969; Depla, ten Have, van Balkom, & de Graaf, 2008; Huppert, Grill, & Brandt, 2013). Despite substantial variability in adults, fear of heights in infants is described as a universal development, akin to language acquisition or learning to walk. Indeed, some researchers claim that fear of heights is innate (Menzies & Clarke, 1993; Poulton et al., 1998). From an evolutionary standpoint, avoidance of falling-off places is undoubtedly adaptive, making accounts of innate or early developing height fear popu- lar and satisfying. However, the processes mediating avoidance of a cliff in infancy appear unrelated to the development of fear of heights. Young infants respond flexibly to novel challenges by detecting the fit between the environment and their own abilities. They actively explore, assess, and generate creative alternatives that suit the constraints of the current situation. Far more adaptive than an automatic fear response is the ability to perceive affordances. Although the construct of fear is attractive, it is not necessary to describe the flexible and adaptive behavior of infants at the edge of a drop-off.

Recommended Reading

Adolph, K. E., & Kretch, K. S. (2012). (See References). Describes research on the visual cliff and research inspired by the visual cliff and places the original paradigm and subsequent studies into historical context.

Adolph, K. E., & Robinson, S. R. (2013). (See References). A description of the development of locomotion, from prena- tal movements to mature walking and beyond, relating the phenomena to general conceptual issues of learning and development.

Campos, J. J., Hiatt, D., Ramsay, D., Henderson, C., & Svejda, M. (1978). (See References). A series of studies that explores the relations between locomotor experience and responses to the visual cliff, including the placing and crossing para- digms.

Gibson, E. J., & Walk, R. D. (1960). (See References). The clas- sic article reporting the first comparative experiments on the visual cliff, including several animal species and human infants.

Kretch, K. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2013a). (See References). A recent study using an actual, adjustable cliff that provides evidence against fear of heights in infancy.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.

Funding

The project was supported by Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Grant R37HD033486 to Karen E. Adolph. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National

at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

http://cdp.sagepub.com/

Fear of Heights in Infants? 65

Institute of Child Health and Human Development or the National Institutes of Health.

References

Adolph, K. E. (1997). Learning in the development of infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62(3, Serial No. 251).

Adolph, K. E. (2000). Specificity of learning: Why infants fall over a veritable cliff. Psychological Science, 11, 290–295.

Adolph, K. E., & Avolio, A. M. (2000). Walking infants adapt locomotion to changing body dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1148–1166.

Adolph, K. E., Berger, S. E., & Leo, A. J. (2011). Developmental continuity? Crawling, cruising, and walking. Developmental Science, 14, 306–318.

Adolph, K. E., Karasik, L. B., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2010). Using social information to guide action: Infants’ locomo- tion over slippery slopes. Neural Networks, 23, 1033–1042.

Adolph, K. E., & Kretch, K. S. (2012). Infants on the edge: Beyond the visual cliff. In A. Slater & P. Quinn (Eds.), Developmental psychology: Revisiting the classic studies (pp. 36–55). London, England: SAGE.

Adolph, K. E., & Robinson, S. R. (2013). The road to walking: What learning to walk tells us about development. In P. Zelazo (Ed.), Oxford handbook of developmental psychol- ogy (pp. 403–443). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Adolph, K. E., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Ishak, S., Karasik, L. B., & Lobo, S. A. (2008). Locomotor experience and use of social information are posture specific. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1705–1714.

Agras, S., Sylvester, D., & Oliveau, D. (1969). The epidemiology of common fears and phobia. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 10, 151–156.

Bertenthal, B. I., Campos, J. J., & Barrett, K. C. (1984). Self- produced locomotion: An organizer of emotional, cogni- tive, and social development in infancy. In R. N. Emde & R. J. Harmon (Eds.), Continuities and discontinuities in development (pp. 175–210). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. C. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149–219.

Campos, J. J., Bertenthal, B. I., & Kermoian, R. (1992). Early experience and emotional development: The emergence of wariness of heights. Psychological Science, 3, 61–64.

Campos, J. J., Hiatt, S., Ramsay, D., Henderson, C., & Svejda, M. (1978). The emergence of fear on the visual cliff. In M. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect (pp. 149–182). New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Depla, M. F. I. A., ten Have, M. L., van Balkom, A. J. L. M., & de Graaf, R. (2008). Specific fears and phobias in the general population: Results from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS). Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43, 200–208.

Gibson, E. J. (1982). The concept of affordances in develop- ment: The renascence of functionalism. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), The concept of development: The Minnesota symposia

on child psychology (Vol. 15, pp. 55–81). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Gibson, E. J. (1991). An odyssey in learning and perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Gibson, E. J., Riccio, G., Schmuckler, M. A., Stoffregen, T. A., Rosenberg, D., & Taormina, J. (1987). Detection of the traversability of surfaces by crawling and walking infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 533–544.

Gibson, E. J., & Schmuckler, M. A. (1989). Going somewhere: An ecological and experimental approach to development of mobility. Ecological Psychology, 1, 3–25.

Gibson, E. J., & Walk, R. D. (1960). The “visual cliff.” Scientific American, 202, 64–71.

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual percep- tion. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Held, R., & Hein, A. (1963). Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 872–876.

Huppert, D., Grill, E., & Brandt, T. (2013). Down on heights? One in three has visual height intolerance. Journal of Neurology, 260, 597–604.

Kretch, K. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2013a). Cliff or step? Posture- specific learning at the edge of a drop-off. Child Development, 84, 226–240.

Kretch, K. S., & Adolph, K. E. (2013b). No bridge too high: Infants decide whether to cross based on bridge width not drop-off height. Developmental Science, 16, 336–351.

LoBue, V. (2013). What are we so afraid of? How early atten- tion shapes our most common fears. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 38–42.

Menzies, R. G., & Clarke, J. C. (1993). The etiology of fear of heights and its relationship to severity and individual response patterns. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 355–365.

Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learning theory perspective on anxiety disorders: It’s not what you thought it was. American Psychologist, 61, 10–269.

Poulton, R., Davies, S., Menzies, R. G., Langley, J. D., & Silva, P. A. (1998). Evidence for a non-associative model of the acquisition of a fear of heights. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 537–544.

Rachman, S. J. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear acqui- sition: A critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375−387.

Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., Camras, L. A., & Witherington, D. (2006). Emotional development: Action, communication, and understanding. In N. Einsenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 226–299). New York, NY: John Wiley.

Salassa, J. R., & Zapala, D. A. (2009). Love and fear of heights: The pathophysiology and psychology of height imbalance. Wilderness & Environmental Medicine, 20, 378–382.

Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effects on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195–200.

at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded from

http://cdp.sagepub.com/

66 Adolph et al.

Sroufe, L. A. (1977). Wariness of strangers and the study of infant development. Child Development, 48, 731–746.

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Adolph, K. E., Lobo, S. A., Karasik, L. B., Dimitropoulou, K. A., & Ishak, S. (2008). When infants take mothers’ advice: 18-month-olds integrate perceptual and social information to guide motor action. Developmental Psychology, 44, 734–746. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.734

Ueno, M., Uchiyama, I., Campos, J. J., Dahl, A., & Anderson, D. I. (2011). The organization of wariness of heights in expe- rienced crawlers. Infancy, 17, 376–392.

Walk, R. D. (1966). The development of depth perception in animals and human infants. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 31(5, Serial No. 107).

Walk, R. D., & Gibson, E. J. (1961). A comparative and ana- lytical study of visual depth perception. Psychological Monographs, 75(15, Whole No. 519).

Waters, E., Matas, L., & Sroufe, L. A. (1975). Infants’ reactions to an approaching stranger: Description, validation, and functional significance of wariness. Child Development, 46, 348–356.

Witherington, D. C., Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Lejeune, L., & Seah, E. (2005). Avoidance of heights on the visual cliff in newly walking infants. Infancy, 7, 285– 298.

Zuckerman, M. (1983). Sensation seeking and sports. Personality and Individual Differences, 4, 285–293.

at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015cdp.sagepub.comDownloaded fro

The Influence of Culturally Bound Syndromes and Worldview on the Counseling Relationship

Please no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resource on your own before you bid. One of the references must come from Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2016). You are expected to include at least one scholarly and peer-reviewed resource outside of those provided in the readings for each discussion post. I need this completed by 04/25/18 at 6pm.  You will need access to  DSM-IV-T.

Please thoroughly read the Discussion Posting and Response Rubric attached to evaluate both the posts and responses. There are four components evaluated for each Discussion Post and Response.

1. Responsiveness to Discussion Question /9

2. Critical Thinking, Analysis, and Synthesis /9

3. Professionalism of Writing /5

4. Responsiveness to Peers /9

To get the highest grade possible, ask yourself if you have SURPASSED the following standards as you re-read your posts BEFORE submitting them:

1. Responsiveness: For the Main Post: Did I answer the entire question?  Is it on time?  Does the answer demonstrate that I have read the material for the week and really thought about it?

2. Critical Thinking, Analysis, and Synthesis: Does my post demonstrate my ability to apply, reflect, AND synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly learning 0bjectives? Have I integrated and mastered the general principles, ideas, and skills presented? Do my reflections include a clear and direct correlation to authentic examples or are they drawn from professional experience? Do my insights demonstrate significant changes in awareness, self-understanding, and knowledge?

3. Professionalism of Writing: Do my posts meet graduate-level writing expectations (e.g., are clear, concise, and use appropriate language; make few errors in spelling, grammar, and syntax; provide information about sources when paraphrasing or referring to it; use a preponderance of original language and directly quote only when necessary or appropriate)? Are my postings courteous and respectful when offering suggestions, constructive feedback, or opposing viewpoints?

The Influence of Culturally Bound Syndromes and Worldview on the Counseling Relationship

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders includes a section that highlights diagnoses that are specific to a particular culture. If a disorder is prevalent among the majority of members of a particular culture, can a counselor assume a client from that culture is likely to incur that disorder? Some have argued that culture-specific diagnoses are actually a barrier to multicultural counseling. To what extent do a counselor’s cultural biases create barriers in counseling?

For this Discussion, review the assigned Learning Resources for this week and refer to Table 10.1: Culture Bound Syndromes from the DSM-IV-TR in the Sue and Sue course text. Then, with culturally bound syndromes in mind, consider how a counselor’s worldview could influence the diagnosis of a client or the interpretation of the symptoms a client experiences.

With these thoughts in mind:

Post by Day 3 one way a counselor’s cultural worldview might affect his or her assessment and diagnosis of a client’s situation. In addition, explain how assumptions based solely on culture could adversely influence the client and the counseling relationship.

Support your responses with specific references to the Learning Resources and current literature.

Required Resources

Readings

· Hays, P. A. (2016). Addressing cultural complexities in practice: Assessment, diagnosis, and therapy (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

o Chapter 6, “Creating a Positive Therapeutic Alliance” (pp. 101-123)

o Chapter 7, “Conducting a Culturally Responsive Assessment” (pp. 127-160)

o Chapter 8, “Using Standardized Tests in a Culturally Responsive Way” (pp. 161-194)

o Chapter 9, “Making a Culturally Responsive Diagnosis” (pp. 195-223)

· Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2016). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory and practice (7th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

o Chapter 5, “The Impact of Systemic Oppression: Counselor Credibility and Client Worldviews” (pp. 145-177)

o Chapter 7, “Barriers to Multicultural Counseling and Therapy: Individual and Family Perspectives” (pp. 215-249)

o Chapter 8, “Communication Styles and Its Impact on Counseling and Psychotherapy” (pp. 251-281)

o Chapter 10, “Non-Western Indigenous Methods of Healing: Implications for Multicultural Counseling and Therapy” (pp. 321-351)

o Chapter 13, “Culturally Competent Assessment” (pp. 429-455)

· American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

o “Cultural Formation” (pp. 749 –759)

· Document: The Case of Mrs. Hudson (Word document)

Optional Resources

· State University of New York, New Paltz, Institute for Disaster Mental Health. (n.d.). Tip sheet on Haitian culture.

· Desrosiers, A., & St. Fleurose, S. (2002). Treating Haitian patients: Key cultural aspects. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 56(4), 508–521.

· Nicolas, G., DeSilva, A. M., Grey, K. S., & Gonzalez-Eastep, D. (2006). Using a multicultural lens to understand illnesses among Haitians living in America. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 37(6), 702–707.

Culturally Competent Assessment

Please no plagiarism and make sure you are able to access all resource on your own before you bid. One of the references must come from Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2016). You are expected to include at least one scholarly and peer-reviewed resource outside of those provided in the readings for each discussion post. I need this completed by 04/29/18 at 5pm. I have attached the case study.

Application Assignment: Culturally Competent Assessment

Counseling has a history of perpetuating bias through diagnosis and assessment. In this Application Assignment, you apply cultural competence to a counseling assessment.

For this Application Assignment, review the case study “The Case of Mrs. Hudson” and the description of culturally relevant assessment in the course text Addressing Cultural Complexities in Practice. Consider the case study in terms of a culturally competent diagnosis. Think about the issues involved in culturally competent assessment and their impact on counseling.

The Assignment:

With the case study in mind, in a 2- to 3-page paper:

· Develop a culturally relevant assessment of symptoms of the client in the case study. Focus on the concerns specific to Mrs. Hudson’s culture.

· Summarize the major issues in conducting a culturally competent assessment and explain how they might affect the process and progress of counseling.

Support your Application Assignment with specific references to all resources used in its preparation.

Note: Sue and Sue (2013) have not updated their text with the new emphases on culture and gender as presented in the DSM-5. Walden is working to provide updated research given the changes in the DSM-5as materials become available. However, please note that the DSM-5 provides a section about client factors related to culture and gender that could impact diagnoses. Please refer to this section in the DSM-5(pp. 749–759) as a reference for this Assignment. These additions provide a stronger opportunity for clinicians to assess their clients more thoroughly than in the previous DSM-IV-TR.

Required Resources

Readings

· Hays, P. A. (2016). Addressing cultural complexities in practice: Assessment, diagnosis, and therapy (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

o Chapter 6, “Creating a Positive Therapeutic Alliance” (pp. 101-123)

o Chapter 7, “Conducting a Culturally Responsive Assessment” (pp. 127-160)

o Chapter 8, “Using Standardized Tests in a Culturally Responsive Way” (pp. 161-194)

o Chapter 9, “Making a Culturally Responsive Diagnosis” (pp. 195-223)

· Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (2016). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory and practice (7th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

o Chapter 5, “The Impact of Systemic Oppression: Counselor Credibility and Client Worldviews” (pp. 145-177)

o Chapter 7, “Barriers to Multicultural Counseling and Therapy: Individual and Family Perspectives” (pp. 215-249)

o Chapter 8, “Communication Styles and Its Impact on Counseling and Psychotherapy” (pp. 251-281)

o Chapter 10, “Non-Western Indigenous Methods of Healing: Implications for Multicultural Counseling and Therapy” (pp. 321-351)

o Chapter 13, “Culturally Competent Assessment” (pp. 429-455)

· American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

o “Cultural Formation” (pp. 749 –759)

· Document: The Case of Mrs. Hudson (Word document)

Optional Resources

· State University of New York, New Paltz, Institute for Disaster Mental Health. (n.d.). Tip sheet on Haitian culture.

· Desrosiers, A., & St. Fleurose, S. (2002). Treating Haitian patients: Key cultural aspects. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 56(4), 508–521.

· Nicolas, G., DeSilva, A. M., Grey, K. S., & Gonzalez-Eastep, D. (2006). Using a multicultural lens to understand illnesses among Haitians living in America. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 37(6), 702–707.